benachmuntaner_asociación clase social y estratificación

Upload: juan-camilo-arboleda-sarmiento

Post on 02-Jun-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    1/11

    The two major variables used to operationalize socioeconomic

    position in studies of social inequalities in health are social

    stratification and social class. Social stratification refers to theranking of individuals along a continuum of economic or cultural

    attributes such as income or years of education. These rankings

    are known as simple gradational measures.1 Most social epi-

    demiologists use several measures of social stratificationsimultaneously because single measures have been insufficient

    to account for social inequalities in the health of populations.

    Measures of social stratification are important predictors of pat-

    terns of mortality and morbidity,2 and during the last decade,

    a number of investigations of social inequalities in health have

    assessed the relation between indicators of social stratification and

    health outcomes. However, despite their usefulness in predicting

    health outcomes, these measures do not reveal the social mech-

    anisms that explain how individuals arrive at different levels

    of economic, political, and cultural resources,3 in part perhaps

    IJE vol.32 no.6 International Epidemiological Association 2003; all rights reserved. International Journal of Epidemiology 2003;32:950958

    DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyg170

    SPECIAL THEME: MENTAL HEALTH

    The associations of social class and social

    stratification with patterns of general andmental health in a Spanish populationCarles Muntaner,1 Carme Borrell,2 Joan Benach,3 M Isabel Pasarn2 and Esteve Fernandez4

    Accepted 28 February 2003

    Background Social class, as a theoretical framework, represents a complementary approach to

    social stratification by introducing social relations of ownership and control over

    productive assets to the analysis of inequalities in economic, political, and

    cultural resources. In this study we examined whether measures of social class

    were able to explain and predict self-reported general and mental health over

    and above measures of social stratification.

    Methods We tested this using the Barcelona Health Interview Survey, a cross-sectional

    survey of 10 000 residents of the citys non-institutionalized population in 2000.

    We used Erik Olin Wrights indicators of social class position, based on ownership

    and control over productive assets. As measures of social stratification we used

    the Spanish version of the British Registrar General (BRG) classification, and

    education. Health-related variables included self-perceived health and mental

    health as measured by Goldbergs questionnaire.

    Results Among men, high level managers and supervisors reported better health than all

    other classes, including small business owners. Low-level supervisors reported

    worse mental health than high-level managers and non-managerial workers,

    giving support to Wrights contradictory class location hypothesis with regard tomental health. Social class indicators were less useful correlates of health and

    mental health among women.

    Conclusions Our findings highlight the potential health consequences of social class positions

    defined by power relations within the labour process. They also confirm that

    social class taps into parts of the social variation in health that are not captured

    by conventional measures of social stratification and education.

    Keywords Social class, social stratification, socioeconomic status, mental health, self-rated health

    1 Department of Behavioral and Community Health Nursing and Department

    of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, University of Maryland at

    Baltimore, USA.2 Municipal Institute of Public Health of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.3 Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain.4 Institut Catal dOncologia, Barcelona, Spain.

    Correspondence: Carles Muntaner Bonet, Suite 645/BCH, University of

    Maryland-Baltimore, 655 West Lombard Street, Baltimore, MD 21201, USA.

    E-mail: [email protected]

    950

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    2/11

    because they have generally been selected for pragmatic

    considerations, i.e. availability of data, rather than for theoretical

    reasons.

    Social class is defined by relations of ownership or control over

    productive resources (i.e. physical, financial, organizational).

    Social class has important consequences for the lives of indi-

    viduals: the extent of an individuals legal right and power to

    control productive assets determines an individuals strategiesand practices devoted to acquire income and, as a result, deter-

    mines the individuals standard of living.1 Thus the class position

    of business owner compels its members to hire workers and

    extract labour from them, while the worker class position

    compels its members to find employment and perform labour.

    Although there have been few empirical studies of social class

    and health, the need to study social class has been noted by

    social epidemiologists.2,4 Social class provides an explicit

    relational mechanism (property, management) that explains

    how economic inequalities are generated and how they may

    affect health. For example, in a recent study,5 a team of US

    epidemiologists found that low-level supervisors, who could

    hire and fire front line personnel but did not have policy or

    decision-making authority in the firm, showed higher rates ofdepression and anxiety disorders than both upper management

    (who had authority and decision-making attributes) and non-

    management workers (who had neither). This finding was

    predicted by the contradictory class location hypothesis (super-

    visors are in conflict with both workers and upper management

    and do not have control over policy) but was not predicted or

    explained by indicators of years of education or income gradients.

    Moreover, the income hypothesis would have failed to provide

    a mechanism and would have led to the expectation that super-

    visors, because of their higher incomes, would present lower

    rates of anxiety and depression than workers. A handful of

    studies in psychiatric epidemiology57 suggest that social strati-

    fication and social class are not equivalent; rather, they capture

    different parts of the social variation in population mentalhealth. Therefore, the purpose of our study was to examine the

    relationships between measures of social stratification (education,

    British Registrar General Classification [BRG]), measures of

    social class (Wrights social class indicators, i.e. relationship to

    productive assets), and indicators of general health and mental

    health.

    The measures of social class used in our investigation

    originate from a social class model that has been accumulating

    empirical support over the last 20 years (e.g. refs 814). Wrights

    social class indicators assess ownership of productive assets, and

    control and authority relations in the workplace (control over

    organizational assets1). Property rights over the financial or

    physical assets used in the production of goods and services

    generate three class positions: employers, who are self-employed and hire labour; the traditional petit bourgeoisie, who

    are self-employed but do not hire labour; and workers who sell

    their labour.1 These social class positions reflect the relational

    properties underlying economic inequality.15 Indicators of

    productive asset ownership gauge a relational mechanism that

    generates economic inequality (i.e. deriving income from

    owning property). Both neo-material16 and psychological5

    mechanisms suggest that owners might present better overall

    health and mental health than workers. Large property owners

    tend to be wealthier17 than others and thus might be expected

    to experience the greater material well-being that is conducive

    to better health.3 In addition, large owners enjoy the predict-

    ability and control in life that are predictive of better mental

    health.18 They are not subject to the stressors of unpredict-

    ability and lack of control associated with relying exclusively on

    salaries or wages for income. As a result, they may enjoy better

    health. Even small property owners can derive economic security

    from wealth, which is more concentrated among propertyowners than income.19,20 However, since most small businesses

    go bankrupt, the suitability of this hypothetical mechanism to

    small capitalist class positions is less evident.21 These hypothetical

    mechanisms linking property ownership to economic security

    were part of the underlying rationale for this study.

    Control over organizational assets (power and control in the

    workplace) is determined by two kinds of relations at work:

    (1) influence over company policy (e.g. making decisions over

    number of people employed, products or services delivered,

    amount of work performed, size and distribution of budgets);

    and (2) sanctioning authority (granting or preventing pay raises

    or promotions, hiring, firing, or temporally suspending a sub-

    ordinate).1 The supervisory and policy making functions of

    managers allow them to enjoy greater wealth than workers,for example, through income derived from shares of stock,

    incentives, bonuses, and hierarchical pay scales.22 As a conse-

    quence, we anticipate that managers will present better health

    and mental health than non-managers, in accordance with the

    hypothesis derived from asset ownership. Furthermore,

    workplace authority relations add another mechanism that may

    impact health, i.e. control over ones work and the ability to

    extract labour effort from others, increasing ones sense of control

    and predictability at work.23 Indeed, the work organization

    literature,23 including the Whitehall study,24 suggests that in

    addition to greater access to income, wealth, and job security,

    control over work may be a mechanism linking managerial class

    positions to better health.

    Following Erik Wrights class theory, we defined andmeasured managerial class positions according to policy-making

    power within the labour process and supervisory functions over

    others labour. Social stratification (e.g. occupational categories)

    does not define or measure relational mechanisms within the

    labour process. Popular stratification measures such as occu-

    pational groups cannot generate specific hypotheses because

    they are compatible with many potential mechanisms (e.g.

    occupational prestige categories, income, authority).

    According to Wrights contradictory class locations hypoth-

    esis, supervisors are in a special position (i.e. a contradictory

    class position in production relations9), subjected both to the

    pressure of upper management to discipline the workforce and

    the antagonism of subordinate workers, while exerting little

    influence over company policy.1 This situation may exposesupervisors to high demands and low control at work, which

    are risk factors for mental disorders.5 Therefore, supervisors are

    more likely to present poorer mental health than managers.

    Wright includes skills/credentials relations as part of his map

    of class positions (Figure 1, the expert, semi-skilled, and un-

    skilled class positions1). Experts are defined as those holding

    jobs that require skills, particularly accredited credentialed

    skills, which are scarce relative to their demand by the market.

    Experts enjoy a credential rent: their wages are usually above

    the cost of the reproduction of their training.9 Semi-skilled and

    SOCIAL STATUS AND HEALTH IN SPAIN 951

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    3/11

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    4/11

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    5/11

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    6/11

    bivariate regressions. However, in multivariate Model 1(adjusted by education), the only associations that remained

    significant were those involving semi-skilled workers and those

    with less than primary education; and in Model 2 only BRG

    class V remained associated with poor mental health (Table 5).

    As an alternative to Wrights detailed social class structure,

    we explored social class relations of property, organizational,

    and credential assets separately from each other. Results indicate

    that poor health status is associated with supervisor, non-

    managerial, semi-skilled, and unskilled class positions among

    men and with semi-skilled and unskilled class positions among

    women. Poor mental health was also associated with semi-

    skilled and unskilled class positions among women. Among

    both men and women, credentials are associated with general

    health after adjustment for education and BRG. Having acredentialed occupation is protective of health over and above

    the amount of education needed to gain access to that kind of

    occupation. That is, the same amount of education would not

    protect a persons health that much if that person did not use it

    to gain a more advantaged social class.

    Discussion

    The findings from the Barcelona 2000 HIS support several of

    our hypotheses with regard to the relationships between social

    class and health. Among men, managers and supervisors withhigh credentials had better self-perceived health than men in

    other class positions, most notably semi-skilled and unskilled

    workers, semi-skilled and unskilled supervisors, petit bourgeois,

    and small employers. Although the findings for women were

    consistent with those for men, the managerial (i.e. organ-

    izational assets) hypothesis could not be confirmed among

    women or for mental health outcomes, except for low-level

    supervisors, due to large CI, particularly in multivariate models.

    Neither social stratification nor social class are related to mental

    health in men. This is commonly found with the GHQ.3436 It

    is important to note that although social class was not associated

    with mental health, the stratification measures, including the

    education measure, were not associated with mental health

    either. On the other hand, among women both measures ofsocial stratification were associated with mental health.

    Among those in capitalist class positions, poor self-perceived

    health was rare; however, the small number of representatives

    of this class in our sample (46 men and 16 women) reduced the

    power of tests involving ownership relations in multivariate

    analyses. This problem has also been noted in sociological

    surveys.37 In addition, as shown in a British survey on class

    structure,38 capitalists who participate in general surveys are

    more prone to be misclassified than members of other class

    positions, and further the wealth and power held by large

    SOCIAL STATUS AND HEALTH IN SPAIN 955

    Table 4 Bivariate and multivariate associations between poor self-perceived health status, social class, and social stratification. Working men and

    women population, 1664 years old. Barcelona 2000

    Men Women

    Bivariatea Model 1 Model 2 Bivariatea Model 1 Model 2

    Variables OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI

    EO Wright classification

    Capitalists 2.39 0.49, 11.7 1.99 0.40, 9.86 1.93 0.38, 9.83 1.95 0.37, 10.29 1.60 0.30, 8.60 1.44 0.26, 8.13

    Small employers 5.86 2.14, 16.02 4.22 1.50, 11.87 4.39 1.46, 13.2 1.92 0.69, 5.30 1.28 0.43, 3.78 1.09 0.34, 3.42Petit bourgeoisie 4.49 1.63, 12.36 2.98 1.04, 8.50 2.92 0.95, 8.93 1.41 0.51, 3.93 0.92 0.31, 2.73 0.63 0.20, 2.03

    Managers experts and

    semi-skilled,

    supervisors experts 1 1 1 1 1 1

    Supervisors

    semi-skilled 6.55 2.33, 18.45 4.97 1.72, 14.37 5.05 1.60, 15.9 1.14 0.30, 4.39 1.04 0.27, 4.04 0.78 0.19, 3.18

    Supervisors unskilled 6.64 2.30, 19.19 4.15 1.36, 12.66 4.41 1.33, 14.6 2.74 0.95, 7.91 1.78 0.56, 5.67 1.27 0.37, 4.36

    Workers experts 2.20 0.70, 6.87 2.34 0.75, 7.32 2.50 0.79, 7.90 0.63 0.19, 2.07 0.64 0.19, 2.07 0.54 0.16, 1.81

    Workers semi-skilled 4.91 1.87, 12.83 3.43 1.27, 9.24 3.41 1.16, 10.0 1.79 0.72, 4.47 1.50 0.59, 3.80 1.11 0.40, 3.08

    Workers unskilled 7.63 2.99, 19.47 4.49 1.65, 12.16 4.37 1.47, 13.0 3.29 1.38, 7.82 2.00 0.75, 5.32 1.25 0.43, 3.65

    2 log likelihood 1464.8 (P 0.00b) 1464.1 (P 0.00b) 1459.0 (P 0.00b) 1376.2 (P 0.00b) 1376.2 (P 0.00b) 1376.2 (P 0.00b)

    British Registrar General classification

    I 1 1 1 1

    II 0.75 0.35, 1.59 0.55 0.25, 1.21 1.57 0.75, 3.27 1.56 0.72, 3.38

    III 2.16 1.34, 3.48 1.19 0.65, 2.18 2.37 1.30, 4.32 1.74 0.83, 3.68

    IV 3.40 2.15, 5.39 1.96 1.07, 3.56 4.53 2.49, 8.24 3.39 1.58, 7.28V 4.11 2.15, 7.86 2.19 1.00, 4.76 6.19 3.31, 11.59 4.54 2.02, 10.2

    2 log likelihood 1450.3 (P 0.00b) 1437.1 (P 0.00c) 1359.6 (P 0.00b) 1345.9 (P 0.00c)

    Educational level

    University 1 1 1 1

    Secondary 1.88 1.24, 2.84 1.28 0.79, 2.07 2.00 1.34, 2.98 1.29 0.75, 2.22

    Primary 3.25 2.23, 4.74 2.15 1.35, 3.40 2.65 1.81, 3.87 1.68 0.98, 2.88

    Less than primary 3.82 2.22, 6.60 2.58 1.41, 4.73 6.78 3.99, 11.54 4.43 2.31, 8.51

    2 log likelihood 1462.1 (P 0.00b) 1445.1 (P 0.00c) 1368.5 (P 0.00b) 1350.3 (P 0.00c)

    a Different regressions models for each variable.b P-value of the model.c P-value of adding the new variable (BRG classification or educational level) to the model after inclusion of other measures.

    Age was also included in the models.

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    7/11

    employers makes them less likely to be reached or to be

    motivated to participate in surveys. This may be a limitation ofcontemporary survey research that could be overcome with

    qualitative research (e.g. ref. 39). The poor health of the petit

    bourgeois in our sample could reflect the competition (and high

    rates of business failure) that this social class typically experi-

    ences, especially in the 21st century marketplace dominated by

    large corporations.1,8,10 Similar results were described by

    Benach et al.40 Using the 3rd European Survey on Working

    Conditions, they found that in the European Union (EU) small

    employers were at greater risk of reporting high levels of stress

    and fatigue and low dissatisfaction and absenteeism.

    Interestingly, in our study men in low-level supervisory class

    positions (i.e. unskilled supervisors) showed a higher rate of

    poor mental health than semi-skilled and unskilled workers.

    This is consistent with the notion that contradictory classrelations are mentally hazardous. Multivariate results showing

    that unskilled supervisors, but not semi-skilled or unskilled

    workers, were more likely to present poor mental health

    than managers and expert supervisors, are also consistent

    with this. Low-level supervisors are the de facto management

    to workers, while simultaneously occupying the position of

    workers in relation to upper management, and they are in

    conflict with both.9 These findings are consistent with the

    results from a survey conducted in Baltimore (USA), in the

    mid 1990s.5

    As expected, experts were found to enjoy better health than

    non-experts. The health consequences of the skill-credentialsdimension may be crucial for individuals occupying dual class

    positions. Scarce credentials (i.e. expertise) confer a notable

    health benefit to low-level supervisors. However, because Wrights

    indicators of skills/credentials are similar to occupational strati-

    fication, it is unclear whether the skill/credential measure is

    actually a measure of social stratification or a measure of social

    class proper.

    The finding that credentialed occupations are protective of

    health over and above the amount of education needed to gain

    access to that occupation is consistent with a materialist rather

    than a psychosocial interpretation.16

    In addition to the limitations of cross-sectional data for draw-

    ing causal inferences, our survey was restricted to the employed

    population and thus we were not able to ascertain the social classpositions of those working outside the labour market, such as the

    mediated class positions of family members (dependent elderly,

    children, housewives devoted to unpaid household labour), or

    class trajectory positions (students). This is important mainly for

    women because in Spain the participation of women in the

    labour market is lower than in other countries of the EU (in

    1998 womens activity rate was 37%, whereas the mean of the

    EU was 46%).41 Although the survey was large, some lack of

    power was noted in using Wrights class scheme given the number

    of categories it contains. In future research involving general

    956 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY

    Table 5 Bivariate and multivariate associations between poor mental health status, social class and social stratification. Working men and women

    population, 1664 years old. Barcelona 2000

    Men Women

    Bivariatea Model 1 Model 2 Bivariatea Model 1 Model 2

    Variables OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI

    EO Wright classification

    Capitalists 1.62 0.48, 5.45 1.54 0.45, 5.26 1.69 0.49, 5.87 1.52 0.25, 9.21 1.34 0.22, 8.25 1.60 0.25, 9.99

    Small employers 1.87 0.85, 4.10 1.79 0.79, 4.03 1.98 0.83, 4.73 2.74 1.04, 7.24 2.16 0.78, 5.95 2.51 0.88, 7.17Petit bourgeoisie 1.77 0.80, 3.88 1.69 0.74, 3.86 1.71 0.71, 4.14 2.08 0.78, 5.57 1.72 0.62, 4.79 1.69 0.58, 4.85

    Managers experts and

    semi-skilled,

    supervisors experts 1 1 1 1 1 1

    Supervisors

    semi-skilled 1.77 0.76, 4.10 1.67 0.70, 3.97 1.82 0.71, 4.65 2.08 0.66, 6.55 1.97 0.62, 6.21 1.98 0.61, 6.41

    Supervisors unskilled 2.99 1.35, 6.65 2.80 1.18, 6.65 3.28 1.27, 8.51 2.95 1.08, 8.03 2.25 0.77, 6.58 2.55 0.84, 7.78

    Workers experts 1.30 0.57, 2.97 1.32 0.58, 3.03 1.17 0.50, 2.75 1.48 0.56, 3.90 1.52 0.58, 4.01 1.35 0.51, 3.62

    Workers semi-skilled 1.90 0.96, 3.77 1.82 0.89, 3.73 1.89 0.85, 4.19 2.65 1.12, 6.28 2.44 1.02, 5.82 2.40 0.95, 6.02

    Workers unskilled 1.79 0.92, 3.49 1.69 0.80, 3.50 1.92 0.83, 4.44 2.57 1.11, 5.93 1.91 0.76, 4.79 1.94 0.73, 5.14

    2 log likelihood 1394.3 (P= 0.42b) 1393.0(P= 0.42b) 1386.2 (P= 0.42b) 1563.4 (P= 0.21b) 1563.4 (P= 0.21b) 1563.4 (P= 0.21b)

    British Registrar General classification

    I 1 1 1 1

    II 1.64 0.96, 2.79 1.37 0.76, 2.46 1.50 0.88, 2.57 1.33 0.76, 2.35

    III 1.40 0.89, 2.22 0.91 0.50, 1.65 1.29 0.82, 2.04 1.02 0.58, 1.79

    IV 1.40 0.89, 2.20 0.96 0.53, 1.73 1.91 1.21, 3.02 1.55 0.86, 2.81

    V 1.80 0.91, 3.57 1.18 0.52, 2.66 2.35 1.39, 3.97 2.01 1.03, 3.93

    2 log likelihood 1391.2 (P= 0.46b) 1383.4 (P= 0.58c) 1565.6 (P 0.00b) 1550.8 (P= 0.01c)

    Educational level

    University 1 1 1 1

    Secondary 1.44 1.00, 2.07 1.17 0.75, 1.83 1.56 1.13, 2.15 1.46 0.93, 2.28

    Primary 1.27 0.88, 1.84 1.03 0.65, 1.64 1.28 0.91, 1.79 1.22 0.76, 1.95

    Less than primary 1.15 0.57, 2.34 0.94 0.44, 2.01 2.21 1.27, 3.84 2.10 1.10, 4.00

    2 log likelihood 1400.2 (P= 0.40b) 1392.0 (P= 0.83c) 1571.1 (P= 0.02b) 1557.0 (P= 0.09c)

    a Different regressions models for each variable.b P-value of the model.c P-value of adding the new variable (BRG classification or educational level) to the model after inclusion of the other measures.

    Age was also included in the models.

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    8/11

    SOCIAL STATUS AND HEALTH IN SPAIN 957

    population samples, the number of categories in Wrights class

    framework should perhaps be reduced. Additionally, in studies of

    social class inequalities in health, specific class positions (managers,

    capitalists) may need to be over-sampled, just as ethnic and

    racial minorities must be over-sampled.

    Among men, neither the occupation-based BRG nor education

    seems to be a better predictor of self-perceived health than social

    class. Among women, we did not obtain strong associations. How-ever, non-measured aspects of gender (exposure to worse working

    conditions and lack of access to labour markets; household labour

    and social networks9,14,42) could account for these results.

    Our findings add to the literature on comparative indicators of

    social inequalities and highlight the importance of control over

    material resources,4347 or in Wrights terms, control over organ-

    izational assets. Our results confirm recent studies57 in which

    social class, understood as a social relation of ownership or control

    over productive assets, explains some aspects of the variation in

    health outcomes, while social stratification explains others.

    We can draw several conclusions from this study. Our find-

    ings suggest that surveys in social epidemiology could benefit

    from over-sampling large employers in order to assess the

    health impact of capitalist class positions, which are poorlyrepresented in general population samples.38,48 The poor

    mental health found among low-level supervisors, replicating a

    previous study, suggests that inquiry into the mental health

    effects of contradictory class positions may be a fruitful venue

    for future research. Furthermore, our study findings indicate that

    control over organizational assets, as captured by the power to

    hire and fire labour and decision-making power over company

    policy, may be an important determinant of social inequalities

    in health. Thus, our findings highlight the potential health

    consequences of social class positions defined by power

    relations within the labour process. They also confirm that

    social class taps into parts of the social variation in health that

    are not captured by conventional measures of social stratification

    and education.

    Acknowledgements

    This study was supported with funds from the Municipal

    Institute of Public Health of Barcelona, Spain. The authors want

    to thank Wylbur Hadden for his helpful comments.

    References1 Wright EO. Class Counts: Comparative Studies in Class Analysis .

    Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000.

    2 Lynch J, Kaplan G. Socioeconomic position. In: Berkman Lf, Kawachi I

    (eds). Social Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press, 2000.

    3Muntaner C, Eaton WW, Diala CC. Socioeconomic Inequalities inmental health: a review of concepts and underlying assumptions.

    Health 2000;47:204353.

    4 Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss N. Measuring social class in US public

    health research: concepts, methodologies and guidelines. Ann Rev

    Public Health 1997;18:34178.

    5 Muntaner C, Eaton WW, Diala CC, Kessler RC, Sorlie PD. Social class,

    assets, organizational control and the prevalence common groups of

    psychiatric disorders. Soc Sci Med 1998;47:204353.

    6 Wohlfarth T. Socioeconomic inequality and psychopathology: are

    socioeconomic status and social class interchangeable? Soc Sci Med

    1997;45:399410.

    7 Wohlfarth T, van den Brink W. Social class and substance use

    disorders: the value of social class as distinct from socioeconomic

    status. Soc Sci Med 1998;47:5158.

    8 Wright EO, Singleman J. Proletarization in the changing American

    class structure.Am J Sociol1982;88:176209.

    9 Wright EO. Classes. London: Verso, 1985.

    10 Steinmetz G, Wright EO. The fall and rise of the Petty bourgeoisie:

    changing patterns of employment in the postwar United States. Am JSociol1989;94:9731018.

    11 Wright EO, Cho D. The relative permeability of class boundaries to

    cross class friendships: a comparative study of the United States,

    Canada, Sweden, and Norway.Am Soc Rev1992;57:85102.

    12 Muntaner C, Wolyniec P, McGrath J, Pulver AE. Psychotic inpatients

    social class and their first admission to state or private psychiatric

    Baltimore hospitals.Am J Public Health 1994;84:28789.

    13 Western M, Wright EO. The permeability of class boundaries to

    intergenerational mobility among men in the United States, Canada,

    Norway and Sweden.Am Soc Rev 1994;59:60629.

    14 Wright EO, Baxter J, Birkelund GE. The gender gap in workplace

    authority: a cross-national study.Am Soc Rev 1995;60:40735.

    15 Wright EO. Race, class, and income inequality. Am J Sociol 1978;

    83:136897.

    16 Lynch J, Due P, Muntaner C, Smith GD. Social capital, is it a good

    investment strategy for public health? J Epidemiol Community Health

    2000;54:40408.

    17 Keister L. Wealth in America. Trends in Wealth Inequality . Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press, 2000.

    18 Turner RJ, Roszell P. Psychosocial resources and the stress process. In:

    WR Avison, IH Gotlib. Stress and Mental Health. New York: Plenum,

    1994, pp. 179212.

    19 Wolff EN. Top Heavy: a Study of Wealth Inequity in America. New York:

    20th Century Fund, 1996.

    20 Spilerman S. Wealth and stratification processes. Ann Rev Sociol

    2000;26:497524.

    21 Keister LA. Financial markets, money, and banking. Ann Rev Sociol

    2002;28:3961.

    22 Halaby CN, Weakliem DL. Ownership and authority in the earningsfunctions.Am Soc Rev 1993;58:1630.

    23 Karasek K, Theorell T. Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity and the

    Reconstruction of Working Life. NY: Basic Books, 1990.

    24 Marmot MG, Davey Smith G, Stansfeld et al. Health inequalities

    among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet 1991;

    337:138793.

    25 Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of

    twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav 1997;38:2137.

    26 Goldberg D. The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire. London:

    Oxford University Press, 1972.

    27 Kaplan G, Baron-Epel O. What lies behind the subjective evaluation

    of health status? Soc Sci Med(In press).

    28 Hennessy CH, Moriarty DG, Zack MM, Scherr PA, Brackbill R.

    Measuring health-related quality of life for public health surveillance.Public Health Rep 1994;109:66572.

    29 Borrell C, Rue M, Pasarn MI et al. Trends in social class inequalities in

    health status, health-related behaviors, and health services utilization

    in a southern European urban area (19831994). Prev Med 2000;

    31:691701.

    30 Ajuntament de Barcelona. Encuesta de Poblacin Activa del Instituto

    Nacional de Estadstica, 2001.

    31 Carabaa J, de Francisco A (eds). Teoras Contemporneas De Las Clases

    Sociales. Madrid: Editorial Pablo Iglesias, 1994.

    32 Grupo SEE y Grupo SEMfYC. Una propuesta de medida de la clase

    social.Atencin Primaria 2000;25:35063.

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    9/11

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    10/11

    use a development of the Goldthorpe schema, the National

    Statistics Socio-Economic Classification (NS-SEC).18,19 What

    these have in common are a concern with power and control at

    the workplace: control over ones own working day and work

    career and power over the work of others. Thus, the conceptual

    basis for social class is totally different to that underlying

    stratification. Most importantly, class is an inherently relational

    concept. It is not defined according to an order or hierarchy, butaccording to relations of power and control.

    According to Wright, power or authority are organisational

    assets that allow some workers to benefit from the abilities and

    energies of other workers.17 The hypothetical pathway linking

    class (as opposed to prestige) to health is that some members of

    a work organization are expending less energy and effort and

    getting more (pay, promotions, job security, etc.) in return,

    while others are getting less for more effort. So the less

    powerful are at greater risk of running down their stocks of

    energy and ending up in some kind of physical or psychological

    health deficit. French industrial sociologists called this lusure

    de travailthe usury of work. At the most obvious level, the

    manager sits in an office while the routine workers are exposed

    to all the dangers of heavy loads, dusts, chemical hazards andthe like. But we are all familiar with other situations: the

    academic supervisor who takes credit for the work of a graduate

    student or junior researcher; the manager who takes the credit

    for the efficiency of a secretary. The same process occurs in

    every organization where power is unequal. So the use of the

    Wright schema in health research, as Muntaner et al. point out

    Provides an explicit relational mechanism that explains how

    economic inequalities are generated and how they may affect

    health.

    The authors have used, as their contrast to the Wright class

    measure, not one of the American socioeconomic status

    measures but a Spanish derivation of the British Registrar-

    Generals classification (RGSC). The RGSC has been described

    variously as an indicator of skill level or of general standing inthe community (which sounds like prestige). But there was

    never any system behind the measure, most occupations were

    given the same class as in the previous decennium unless a

    member of the decision making group happened to have

    enough knowledge of any specific one to raise questions. The

    story used to be told about one of the civil servants deciding on

    the class position of a newly emerging occupation on the basis

    of whether he would like his daughter to marry one. This is, of

    course, a perfectly valid method for the allocation of caste

    membership in ethnographic studies. Given that the measure

    has been widely used in Spanish epidemiological studies, and

    that it is used here as an indicator of the RG classification to

    indicate a prestige dimension of stratification, it seems a

    reasonable choice.Muntaner et al. directly address the extent to which the use

    of a specific class measure actually tells us any more than we

    could learn from a measure of income or of prestige by

    considering the health of people in some of Wrights

    contradictory class locations. Supervisors have higher incomes

    and prestige than non-supervisory workers, so on this basis

    their health should be better. However, taking account of the

    relationships between supervisors and other workers might lead

    to a different expectation. There are contradictory pressures on

    supervisors, in that they are subject to the authority of those

    above them but also suffer the antagonism of those below. Sure

    enough, the worst mental (not physical) health in the authors

    analysis was found amongst unskilled supervisors; the group

    thought most likely to have responsibility without very much

    power. This effect seems to have been independent of any

    association between Wright class and education or the Spanish

    version of the RG schema.

    An important point to emerge from the results is the dis-tinction between expertise and education. In fact, the relative

    weakness of education itself as a predictor of health is striking.

    After adjustment for educational level, experts of various kinds

    seem to have relatively low risk of ill-health, both mental and

    physical. The paper is therefore showing us that it is not edu-

    cation per se that favourably influences health, but the access

    education gives to more dominant (or less-dominated) social

    positions. Of course, any examination of the relationships of

    class or education to health needs to take account of the relevant

    national labour market. There may be very large differences

    between the experience of being the owner of a small or medium

    sized company in Spain, the UK, and the US. But in many

    countries, the employment situation of someone designated as

    an expert within a large organization, say a university or gov-ernment bureaucracy, may be little different to an extended

    vacation. The owner/manager of a company employing

    20 people (who counts as a capitalist by the Wright criteria), on

    the other hand, may be consigned to near-slavery by

    comparison.

    Indiscriminate use of measures of social position with multiple

    theoretical bases (or none at all) has hindered progress from the

    description of health inequality towards its explanation. It has

    been all too easy to slip into the kind of lazy thinking that

    proceeds: high social class equals general superiority in lots

    of ways equals better health, what do you expect?. Relational

    social class measures may show weaker associations to some

    health outcomes than prestige or income. But then we will at

    least be able to eliminate some potential causal pathways andconcentrate on those that are better supported by the data.

    References1 Muntaner C. The associations of social class and social stratification

    with patterns of general and mental health in a Spanish population.

    Int J Epidemiol2003;32:95058.

    2 Krieger N, Moss N. Accounting for the publics health: an introduction

    to selected papers from a U.S. conference on measuring social

    inequalities in health. Int J Health Serv1996;26:38390.

    3 Muntaner C, Parsons PE. Income, social-stratification, class, and

    private health-insurancea study of the Baltimore metropolitan-area.

    Int J Health Serv1996;26:65571.4 Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss NE. Measuring social class in US public

    health research: Concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annu Rev

    Public Health 1997;18:34178.

    5 Wohlfarth T. Socioeconomic inequality and psychopathology: are

    socioeconomic status and social class interchangeable? Soc Sci Med

    1997;45:399410.

    6 Wohlfarth T, van den Brink W. Social class and substance use

    disorders: the value of social class as distinct from socioeconomic

    status. Soc Sci Med1998;47:5158.

    7 Bartley M, Sacker A, Firth D, Fitzpatrick R. Understanding social

    variation in cardiovascular risk factors in women and men: the

    SOCIAL STATUS AND HEALTH IN SPAIN 959

  • 8/11/2019 BenachMuntaner_asociacin Clase Social y Estratificacin

    11/11

    advantage of theoretically based measures. Soc Sci Med 1999;

    49:83145.

    8 Duncan GJ, Daly MC, McDonough P, Williams DR. Optimal indicators

    of socioeconomic status for health research. Am J Public Health 2002;

    92:115157.

    9 Hodge RW. The measurement of occupational status. Soc Sci Res

    1981;10:396415.

    10 Duncan OD. A socioeconomic index for all occupations. In: Reiss Jr

    AJ. Occupations and Social Status. New York: Free Press, 1961,

    pp. 10938.

    11 Nam CB, Terrie WE. Measurement of socioeconomic status from

    United States Census data. In: Rossi PH, Nock SL (eds). Measuring

    Social Judgements: The Factorial Survey Approach. Beverley Hills: Sage,

    1982, pp. 95118.

    12 Oakes JM, Rossi PH. The measurement of SES in health research:

    current practice and steps toward a new approach. Soc Sci Med

    2003;56:76984.

    13 Hauser RM, Warren JR. Socioeconomic Indexes for Occupations: A Review,

    Update and Critique. Madison, Wisconsin: Centre for Demography and

    Ecology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1996.

    14 Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH. The Constant Flux. Oxford: Clarendon, 1992.

    15 Evans G, Mills C. In search of the wage-labour/service contract: new

    evidence on the validity of the Goldthorpe class schema. Br J Sociol

    2000;51:64161.

    16 Goldthorpe JH. The Goldthorpe class schema: some observations on

    conceptual and operational issues in relation to the ESRC review of

    government social classification. In: Rose D, OReilly K (eds). Con-

    structing Classes: Toward a New Social Classification for the UK. Swindon:

    ONS/ESRC, 1997, pp. 4048.

    17 Wright EO. Class Counts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,

    1997.

    18 Rose D, Pevalin DJ. The National Statistics Socio-economic Classification:

    Unifying official and sociological approaches to the conceptualisation and

    measurement of social class. ISER working paper no. 20014. Colchester:

    ISER, University of Essex, 2001.

    19 Rose D, Pevalin DJ. The NS-SEC explained. In: Rose D, Pevalin DJ

    (eds). A Researchers Guide to the National Statistics Socio-economic

    Classification . London: Sage, 2003, pp. 2844.

    960 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY