aproximación neo-luriana diagnostico rehabilitación

Upload: jess-morales

Post on 05-Jul-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    1/10

    Neuropsychology  Review, Vol. 9, No. 2, 1999

    A Neo Lurian Approach

     to

     Assessment

     and

     Remediation

    J. P. Das

    1 2

    The first part of this

     article presents

     an operational battery of

     tasks

     for

     measuring

     the  four cognitive

    processes

     of

     Planning, Arousal-Attention,

     and

      Simultaneous

     and

     Successive processing (PASS)

     not

    only  based on the

     qualitative

     data provided in

     Luna's

      syndrome analysis, but also taken  from  tasks

    in

     experimental cognitive

      psychology

      and

      neuropsychology.

     The

      second  part

     of the article presents

    a

     remedial program

     based on

     PASS

     for

     enhancement

     of reading.

     Because this

     part

      provides

     in some

    detail the efficacy of the

     remedial

     procedure, it simultaneously validates the

     PASS

     constructs as well.

    In

     both

     parts of the

      article,

     I have been unmistakably guided by

     Luria's views:

     Tests are

      approaches

    to investigating cognitive  functions, and the purpose of testing is to guide rehabilitation.

    INTRODUCTION

    I had the  good fortune o f  visiting Luria in  October

    and November of  1975. Beyond any doubt, he is the best

    known and the most

     frequently

     cited Russian psychologist

    in

     Western psychological literature. As I wrote in a short

    biography (Das, 1994), Luria remained productive under

    Stalin, did not slow down when removed for a few years

    from the Neurosurgical Institute to the Institute of De-

    fectology,  retained his optimism, and worked extremely

    hard until his death

      following

     a third heart attack. When

    I met Luria less than 2 years before his death, I   found

    that  neuropsychology was thriving under his leadership.

    He

     held clinics three times

      a

     week

     at the

     Neurosurgical

    Institute and through case presentations discussed many

    fine points  of assessing intellectual and emotional prob-

    lems

     as

     well

     as

     methods

     for

     rehabilitation (Das,  1976).

     He

    worked hard until the end of his

     life,

     without any intention

    of retirement, because, as he told me, he did not think that

    mature

     scientists

     should retire and be left to grow roses

    Luria  was born  in Kazan, Russia, o n  July  16 ,  1902,

    and  he died in Moscow on August 14, 1977. His ideas

    have been extremely  helpful  in understanding cognitive

    deficits,

      including dyslexia. While extending Luria's neu-

    1

     University of Alberta,

     Edmonton, Canada.

    2A11  correspondence

     should

     be

     directed

     to the author at Developmental

    Disabilities

      Centre, 6-123D  Education North,  University

      of Alberta,

    Edmonton, AB,

     Canada

     T5G 2E5. E-mail:

     [email protected].

    ropsychological

     findings to the

     area

     of

     intelligence (Das,

    Naglieri,  and

      Kirby,

      1994),  I and my  coworkers  found

    that his syndrome analysis is consistent with a psychome-

    tric analysis of four major cognitive

     processes

     composing

    Planning, Arousal-Attention, and Simultaneous and Suc-

    cessive

     processes (PASS) that were directly derived from

    his

      writings

      on the

      functional

      organization

      of the

      brain.

    Further, Luria's clinical research into neurological

     disor-

    ders and rehabilitation after brain

     injury

     (Luria, 1963)  has

    influenced the construction of a remedial program (PREP)

    for

     ameliorating cognitive deficits, especially those related

    to dyslexia and poor reading. Both of these, that is, con-

    ceptualizing

     intelligence  in terms o f a battery o f

     tests

     and

    constructing a remedial program, are the subject matter of

    this article.

    THE

     PASS THEORY

      IN

     BRIEF

    We must  remember that underlying each method of  psycholog-

    ical investigations  (and

      this

      is also

      true

     of  psychological tests)

    are

     theoretical assumptions

     about the structure of

     psychological

    processes and their breakdown.

     Each

     has its own

     theory,

     its own

    ideology.

     Therefore, any  failure   to  recognize the theory or their

    ideology which lies behind these tests, will quickly lead to disen-

    chantment, and sometimes also to great

     errors.

     (Zeigamik el al.,

    1977, p. 91)

    The  four PASS processes are presented in the con-

    text of PASS theory of intelligence. This theory

      identifies

    three operational units that are important to understanding

    107

    1040-7308/99/0600-0107$16.00/0

     1999  Plenum

     Publishing

     Corporation

    KEY

     WORDS:  Luria;

     neuropsychology;

     assessment; neo-Lurian; PASS; remediation.

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    2/10

    108

    Das

    mental

     functioning:

     Attention, Simultaneous and Succes-

    sive

     processing,

      and

     Planning (Das, Naglieri,

      and Kirby,

    1994). Based on the work of Luria (1966a, 1966b, 1973),

    the PASS theory is grounded in neuropsychology. A neu-

    ropsychological view of intelligence  is

      different from

      the

    existing psychometric tests

      of

     intelligence;

      it

     attempts

      to

    determine

     how the

     mind works

     by

     anchoring

     its functions

    on the brain and by detecting dysfunctions. It

     does

     not

     seek

    to place

     people

     on a scale of

     merit

     and

     therefore

     is not a

     tool

    for

     social selection as IQ tests have been used in the past.

    The maintenance of attention and regulation of cor-

    tical tone,

     the

     processing

      and

     storing

     of information,  and

    the management and direction of mental activity compose

    the activities of the

     functional units that work together

     to

    produce cognitive processing (Das, Naglieri,

      and

     Kirby,

    1994; Luria, 1966b).

    In the schematic diagram of the PASS model shown

    in

     Fig.

      1, there are

      three main divisions: input,

      process-

    ing,

     and output. Input arrives through

     sense

      organs—eyes,

    ears, nose, tongue, and skin—as well as through muscles,

    Fig.  1. Schematic diagram of the

     PASS

     model of  cognitive processes.

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    3/10

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    4/10

     

    Das

    Fig. 2. Scheme for assessment of Planning, Attention, and Simultaneous and Successive processing

    (PASS).

    that

     are

     associated with dyslexia? Several studies suggest

    that this is possible (Das, 1992). The arrangement of tests

    for

     the PASS

     functions

     is given in Fig. 2.

    As

      can be

      seen

      in

      Fig.

      2, at the

      most general lev-

    el, PASS tests measure the four processes: Arousal-Atten-

    tion,

     Simultaneous-Successive processing, and Planning.

    Then,

     for

     each

     process, the

     tests

     are

     named.

     For

     instance,

    Arousal-Attention is divided into two major parts: sus-

    tained attention and selective attention. A task for sus-

    tained

     attention is Auditory Vigilance, in which a man's

    voice

     and a

     woman's voice

     are

     recorded

     on a

     tape recorder

    cassette and played back to the  individual who is being

    tested. The man's voice says the names of five animals

    and

     five

     items

     of furniture.  The

     woman's voice does

      the

    same,

     but the

     animal

     and furniture

     words occur

     at

     random

    at the

     rate

     of one per

     second.

     The

     individual's task

     is to tap

    the table or desk in front of the tester whenever an animal

    word  is

     spoken

      by the man and a

     furniture word

      is

      spo-

    ken by the woman. This is an excellent task not only for

    sustained attention,

     but

     also

     for

     false detections.

     We

     have

    observed that children with attention

     deficit

     are  identified

    more easily

     by

      their

      false

      detection scores than

     by

      their

    correct detections (Das, Mishra, and Kirby, 1994).

    In

     selective attention, we have two kinds of tasks; one

    involves

      selection at the level of reception of the stimu-

    lus,

      and the other at the stage of expression. The first task

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    5/10

    PASS

    111

    consists of a Posner-type test (Posner and Boies, 1971),

    which

     has two p arts. The first consists of letter pairs (BB,

    DD,...)

     that

     ar e

     identical (physical match)

     and

     some dis-

    tractor pairs (BN, TP, ...) ,

      which

      are not. An individu al

    is  to cross out those that "look the same," that is, those

    pairs that

     a re

     physically identical.

     In

     Name M atch,

     on the

    other hand,

      the

      letters, such

     as Bb, Dd, are to be

      crossed

    out—one of them is written in the upper case, whereas the

    other is written in the lower case. This is a receptive atten-

    tion

     test; selective

     attention to the letter

     pairs

     occurs at the

    stage

     of

      presentation.

      In

      contrast, selection

      at the

      expres-

    sion,

      or  response,

      stage

      is

      tested

      by the

      familiar Stroop

    Test (Stroop,

      1935).  The

      last card

      in

      Stroop

      is

      used

      by

    the PASS tests as a measure of interference in selecting to

    respond to the color of the ink, a

      conflict

     card on which

    the four

     color

     words are written in ink color that

     does

     no t

    go  with  the word.

    The

      simultaneous scale

     may

     consist

     of

     Figure M em-

    ory, in which the individual is allowed to look at a geo-

    metrical figure such as a diamond for 5 seconds and then

    find the figure  embedded  in  another geometrical  figure.

    The  other

      simultaneous

      tests  are  Tokens,

      Design Con-

    struction,

      an d

      Matrices, which

     are

     w idely used

      for

     verbal

    (Tokens) and nonverbal spatial

     tests.

     The successive tasks

    include Successive Ordering,

      in

     which

     the

     subject

     is

     asked

    to watch the examiner as he or she turns over little chips;

    then,

      the

      subject

      is

      required

      to

      turn over

      the

      same chips

    in  the  order that th e exam iner did. In Serial Word Recall,

    nine words, all fami liar one-syllable words, are presented

    repeatedly  in  list lengths o f  three to nin e words;  the  sub-

    ject's

     task is to repeat the words in the order in which they

    were presented. Sentence Rep etition

     similarly

     in volves re-

    peating a

     sentence

     that is read to the individ ual ("The p ur-

    ple blued the green yellow"). Speech Rate (Hulme et al.,

    1984)

     is an

     interesting test

      in

     which three words

     are

     given

    to the individual and the subject is required to repeat the

    words aloud as fast as possible ("Say 'egg, bu s, leaf, egg,

    bus,

      leaf, egg, bus, leaf,' on and on,  until  I ask you to

    stop").

     The

     examiner stops

     the

     subject after

      10

     repetitions

    and records the  time that  the  subject

     took

     for  repetition.

    Sentence Questioning

      is an

     extension

      of Sentence

     Repe-

    tition. After

     th e

     subject listens

     to the

      sentence, instead

     of

    repeating

     it, the

     subject mu st answer

      a

     question  from

      the

    sentence ("The pink blued the yellow. Wh o was blued ?").

    The p lanning tasks are the  familiar Visual Search (a

    number  target embedded among

     a field of

     random ly scat-

    tered num bers, a letter target am ong letters, a picture target

    among pictures)

      and

      Trails

      at the low

      level

     of

     planning.

    At the

      medium level

     o f

     planning,

     the

      tasks

      are

     Matching

    Numbers

     (finding

      tw o

     numbers

     that  are

     identical

     in a row

    of six numbers: 5736,  8295,7168,5736,4273,9571) and

    Verbal

     Fluency ("Say

      as

     many words beginning with letter

    d as you

      can").

      At the

     high level

     of

     plann ing, Crack-the-

    Code and

      Planned Composition have been used. Crack-

    the-Code

      is

     like

     th e

     Mastermind game.

      In

      Planned Com-

    position, the subject is show n a vague pictu re and is asked

    to  write  a  story about  it on one  page (see Das, Naglieri,

    and Kirby, 1994,

      for a

     description

     of

      these PASS tasks).

    A

     diagram, presenting

     the

     division

     of the

     PASS tasks

     and

    their  links to perception, memory (mnestic), and concep-

    tualization, is provided  in Fig.  2.

    All  these  tasks are measures of the

      processes

      that

    were previously identified. However, there are two ways

    of evaluating whether or not these tests do measure these

    processes—one

      is

      construct validity,

      and the

      other

      is

    through correlations and factor analysis, both of which

    are discussed in the next section. We prefer construct va-

    lidity  to factor analysis.

    To

      measure attention, simultaneou s and successive

    processing,

      and

     planning,

     the

     instruments must

     be

      consis-

    tent

      with

      both the

     "process

      of

      interest"

      and the theoreti-

    cal foundation  from  which th e  theory  wa s  derived (Das,

    Naglieri,

     and Kirby, 1994, p. 102). Tasks used to measure

    the

      components

      of the

      PASS model

      are

      varied

      in

      terms

    of

     format

     and

     content. Figure

     2

     provides

     a

     brief overview

    of

      selected tasks used to measure each

      functional

      unit of

    the PASS model. Extensive research demonstrating the

    validity of these tasks has been conducted not only by the

    author in his

      lab,

      bu t

      also

      by

      outsiders (Naglieri, 1989;

    Naglieri

     et

     al.,  1989; Naglieri and Reardon,  1993). Many

    of the above tests are now normed and included in the

    Das-Naglieri

     C ognitive Assessment System (1997).

    SYNDROM E ANALYSIS AND MULTIVARIATE

    ANALYSIS

    Of fundamental concern here is the question of the

    possible relationships between Luria's theory and data

    drawn  from

      clinical investigations

      of

      subjects with brain

    damage, and the various investigations as conducted by

    a

      variety of

     researchers

      using primarily multivariate sta-

    tistical methods on data  from  normal and high-incidence

    atypical populations (Das  et

      al.,

      1975, 1979).

      Das and

    Jarman (1991) discussed this issue and suggested that the

    relationship  between these lines of research may be ex-

    amined

      from

      three points

     of

     view:

    1.  To  what extent should these investigations accept

    Luria's model uncritica lly

     a nd

     merely apply

     it in the

    interpretation

     of the

     multivariate data?

    2. To what extent does a logical and empirical relation -

    ship  exist between

      the

      results

     of

      Luria's investiga-

    tions,

     especially syndrom e analysis,

     and the

     results

    from  multivariate studies?

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    6/10

    112

    Das

    3. To

     what extent

     can or

      should Luria's model

      be re-

    vised

     and

     affected

     by the

     outcome

     of

     various multi-

    variate investigations?

    With

     respect

      to the first question, we can find an in-

    creasing acceptance

      of his

      work among neuropsycholo-

    gists

      and the correspond ing grow ing trend toward a .sys-

    tems perspective in neuropsychology (Walsh, 1985,1987).

    Briefly,

     the second question relates to syndrome anal-

    ysis. Its basis may be

      found

      in Luria's clinical technique

    and the interlocking concepts  of a

     functional

      system  and

    a behavioral syndrom e. Luria's te chni que of clinical re-

    search was based on two principles:

    1.

      That

      a

      qualitative analysis

      of

      behavior

     w as

      neces-

    sary,

      utilizing

     virtually

      hundreds

      of

      tasks

      in

      some

    cases, to lead to the

     identification

     of a defect.

    2. That a single lesion or defect may demonstrate itself

    in

      diverse behaviors (Cicerone

      and

      Tupper, 1986).

    The concept of a syndrome, as an extension of these

    principles, was central to Luria's work, whereby a syn-

    drome

     was

     identified

     as a

     constellation

     of

     signs

     and

     symp-

    toms,

     which, in  turn, supplied what we would now term

    an  operational

      definition  for a

      functional

      system. Thus,

    Luria's c linical method entailed the accumulation of con-

    verging evidence

      from

     many tasks to

     define

     a syndrome.

    The syndrome, then,

     emerged

     as a common cognitive pro-

    cess that was disrupted in seemingly disparate tasks. This

    clinical method,

      in

      turn, allowed Luria

     to  define

      various

    areas  of

     regional specialization

      in the

      brain

      and to map

    sets of functional  systems that operate  at various levels of

    generality within and between these areas.

    The research

      process

      that Luria utilized, therefore,

    involved

     the

     definition

     of a

     variety

     of

     syndromes

     by

     iden-

    tifying one or more cognitive processes that are common

    to

     tasks performed

     b y

     subjects

     with

     brain damage.

      A

     par-

    allel approach is to operationally define co nstructs. This is

    achieved

      by  identifying  one or

     more cognitive processes

    that  form

      the

      principal basis

      for

      individual differences

    within

     a set of tasks.

      Such differences among normal

     and

    various atypical subjects

     are

     then supported

     by

     using mul-

    tivariate methods, particularly factor analysis, whenever

    possible.

    The

      approach adopted

      in the

     m ultivariate studies

     by

    Das

      and

      collaborators (Das

      et

      al., 1979; Das, Naglieri,

    and Kirby, 1994) has been to focus mainly on the use of

    tasks that were employed extensively by Luria or tasks

    that

      are very similar to them (although, as noted previ-

    ously, the

     tasks used

     by

     Luria

     are

     many

     and frequently  too

    easy

      for a  nonclinical population), combined with other

    tasks derived

      from

      the  constructs  of  planning,

     arousal-

    attention,

      and so  forth.

      Thus,

      in

      factor analytic terms,

    this  research  is  confirmatory  in two  ways:  the  selection

    of

      tasks and the hypothesized outcomes of the analyses.

    By

     using both a variety of

     tasks

     common to

     Luria's

     inves-

    tigations and a wide selection

      from

     cognitive psychology,

    and

      then

     by

     varying subject populations with known dis-

    orders, research support for the neo-Lurian model, PASS,

    has gradually

     emerged.

     There

     is increasing evidence that

    multivariate investigations

     of

     normal subjects that parallel

    syndrome analysis

     can

     inform theory

      in

     neuropsychology

    (Clark, 1981; Dean,

      1985).

    Among

     the multivariate techniques, a new advance-

    ment,  structural factor analysis, or LISREL, has added

    significant

      power

     to

     traditional factor analysis (McArdle,

    1996).

      This procedure allows hypothesis testing and,

    through path diagrams,

     can

     even suggest causal pathw ays

    between

     cognitive

     processes

     and performance. "Is the ob-

    served syndrome consistent with the  model  of  cognitive

    dysfunction?"

      is the  sort  of  question Luria would have

    asked  and answered had he  been given the  technique fo r

    structural

      factor analysis. However,

      he

      would

      no t

      have

    looked  for a  model  after  doing  the  factor analysis,  but

    before;  nor would he have determined tha t a certain

      test

    is

     good

      for

     diagnosis

     of the

      syndrome merely because

      it

    has a high loading on a factor—for these are

     some

     of the

    pitfalls in

      using

      the

      advanced structural factor analysis,

    as they were  in traditional factor analysis.  If multivariate

    analysis must inform neuropsychological theory, w e can-

    not,

     for

     example, assume

     that a test of

     planning

     or

     attention

    is a

     test

     of that process simply because it loads on the same

    factor as two other

     tests

     o f planning or attention. Clinical

    data obtained

     from

      known

     cases

     of attentional or plan ning

    deficit

     must support the use of the test. Alternatively, the

    test must have been used

     in

     cognitive psychology experi-

    ments

     as a

     test

     of

     attention

     or

     planning.

    The

      last question

      is

      answered

      in the

      context

      of the

    section on Future Directions.

    USING PASS THEORY FOR READING

    REMEDIATION

    When

      primary school children

     of

      otherwise normal

    intelligence  fail  to acquire reading, they are labeled as

    learning

     disabled, poor readers, or dyslexic. In an attempt

    to remediate their problem in word reading, which

     also

    created

     difficulties

      in reading comprehension, we used a

    cognitive

      remedial program, PREP (The PASS Remedial

    Program) (Das,

     1999).

    Remediation  Program: PREP

    The

     PREP

     w as

     designed

      to

     improve

     selected

      aspects

    of

     children's inform ation-processing skills and to increase

    their

     word-reading

     and

     decoding abilities.

     Procedures

     such

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    7/10

    PASS

    113

    as  rehearsal, categorization, monitoring of performance,

    prediction, revision of prediction, word sounding, and

    sound blending are an integral part of each of the PREP

    tasks. Rather tha n being exp licitly taught by the tutor, chil-

    dren receiving training in PREP are encouraged to become

    aware of the use of u nderlying cognitive

     processes.

      This

    is achieved throug h discussion

     of

     what they

     did

     during

     the

    tasks.

     In this w ay, inductive inference and internalization

    occur spontaneously rather than through deductive rule

    learning

      (Campione and Brown, 1987; Vygotsky, 1962).

    The first part of the remediation program consists of

    training tasks tha t challenge the children to use  successive

    or simultaneous processing when required. The objective

    is

      to

      improve children's

      "global"

     processes.

     These

      pre-

    cede

     the

      "bridging" tasks  that  have reading-related

      ma-

    terial. Children are taken through the training tasks in a

    way  that provides structured experience in using the tar-

    geted cognitive process and facilitates the discovery and

    internalization

     of the strategies.

    Transfer occurs as a result of the

     fact

     tha t the surface

    structures of the tasks are diverse and not too much time

    is spent in practicing a single task. "High road"  transfer

    (Salomon

     and

      Perkins, 1989),

      a

     transfer

     of

     principle,

     oc-

    curs in the "bridging" part of this program, as evidenced in

    the study by Das

     et al.

     (1995) an d the new studies reported

    in  the next section. The "global" part is essential for the

    program   to be effective  in imp roving word-decoding per-

    formance, as shown in empirical studies (Das et al.,  1995;

    Das, Naglieri, and Kirby, 1994, chapters 9 and  10).

    The  PREP remediation program comprises  8 tasks,

    each with a global-processing training

     form

     and a curric-

    ulum-related bridging form. The global

     form

     has no read-

    ing

     content,

     but it

     provides

      a

     structured

     series of exercises

    that require  the application  of  successive  and  simultane-

    ous

     strategies

     as well as planning and attentional resource

    allocation.

     The

     bridging form

     has the

     same cognitive

     de-

    mands as its matched global form, but it employs letters

    and their combinations.

    Each task has three levels of   difficulty;  the

      easiest

    level allows

     the

     child

     to

     have initial success with

     the ma-

    terials and to become familiar

     with

     the task and the expec-

    tations

     of the

      training program.

      The

     more  difficult  levels

    build on the

     easiest

      level through added complexity.  De-

    pending on the progress  the child makes, the  remediation

    program typically requires 15 to 18 hours during approx-

    imately

      12

     weeks.

      In

      each session,

     one

      teacher gives

      the

    PREP to  four students.

    Through  its  cognitive-processing  tasks,  the  PREP

    program provides children with an opportunity to develop

    strategies for the key cogn itive processes required for word

    decoding. In contrast to direct practicing of phonemic

    skills,

      the

     cognitive strategies  that  children  with  reading

    disabilities discover and learn by themselves in one part

    of the program are then transferred to word decoding and

    spelling

     in the second part. The PR EP's application for im-

    proving reading skills

     in

     und erachieving children

     is

     briefly

    discussed

      in the

     next section.

    The R emediation

      Studies

    The most recent study of the PREP program, using a

    multistudent format, four  students to one tutor, was done

    by  Carlson and Das

     (1997),

      to improve reading in under-

    achieving ch ildren (the so-called  Chapter 1 children) in

    Hemet, C alifornia. In this study, one teacher instructed

    children in two 50-minute sessions per week during a

    3-month period. Both  the  PREP  N  = 22) and  control

     N  =  15) group samples continued to participate  in the

    regular Chapter 1 programs  offered  in their schools. To ex-

    amine

     th e

      efficacy

      of the

     PREP

      in

     terms

      of

      improvement

    in

      reading achievement, we administered Word Attack

    and  Letter-Word Identification subtests

     of the

      Woodcock

    Reading Mastery Test—Revised (WRMT-R; Woodcock,

    1987) at the beginning and the end of the study in a

    pre/post design. The results showed that the children who

    received PREP remediation gained substantially (almost

    1  year  in  word reading  an d  decoding).  Th e  children  in

    the

     control group

      did not

      show

      any

      improvement. How-

    ever, when comparing the treated and untreated students

    to determine the  efficacy  of PREP training , one must re-

    member that the gains achieved for the group exposed to

    PREP were compared  with the gains of untreated students

    in

      the control group who  were nonetheless receiving  the

    usual remedial education in their

     classroom.

    Although the data  from  the initial Hemet study pro-

    vided support for the

      utility

      of the PREP program in im-

    proving word reading, the number of children in the sam-

    ple was small  N = 22 in the  remedial group, N =  15 in

    the comparison group).

    In

      the second study in Hemet, we asked (a) if the

    improvement due to the PREP could be  replicated with a

    larger group of Chapter 1 children, and (b) if the PREP

    could also  bring about any improvement in some of the

    PASS cognitive

      functions

      underlying word reading and

    decoding.

    As mentioned earlier,

      in

      recent research

      on

      reading

    disability, deficits

     in

     phon ological coding

     and

     articulation

    have been identified as core  difficulties

      associated

      with

    reading disabilities, d espite norm al intelligence (Torgesen

    et  al.,

      1987). We

      have conducted studies that show that

    successive processing is required for phonological coding

    and

      articula tion of sequences of letters and soun ds (Das,

    Mishra, and Kirby, 1994; Kirby and Das,

      1990),

     and that

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    8/10

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    9/10

    PASS 115

    the  functional  organization of higher cortical processes;

    the

     PASS model and the PREP provide a neo-Lurian ex-

    pansion.

    In conclusion, we hope that the PREP program is

    just the beginning of a scientific approach to learning dys-

    functions; it should be

     developed

     to

     remediate math prob-

    lem solving, and even beyond learning disabilities to other

    learning problems such as attention deficit  disorder.

    The gap between a theory of cognitive functions and

    amelioration of cognitive deficits needs to be bridged; our

    approach is one attempt to

     fulfill

      this need. We have tried to

    provide a

     scientific basis

     for

     remediation

     and

     intervention

    and, at the same time, question the rationale of adminis-

    tering standardized intelligence tests.

    Great changes have occurred in the field of intel-

    ligence and the neuropsychology of cognitive processes

    since Luria's time.

     It

     would

     be a

     mistake

     to

     ignore these,

    and Luria would have been the first to advise us against

    adhering to his original notions. The

      greatest

      changes

    have occurred  in our  understanding of the  functions  of

    the frontal  lobes, and Luria longingly suggested that only

    the young scientists who would follow him would be able

    to  discover such functions,  which are planning and con-

    sciousness.

     The

     challenge

     for a

     neo-Lurian theory

     is to in-

    vestigate the highest cognitive functions of humans, which

    was

     begun

     by

     Luria

     and has

     been followed

     by

     Sperry, Shal-

    lice, and others (see Das et al., 1996), and the challenge

    can be met not so

     much

     by

     theorizing,

     but by

     observation

    and more observation, to quote Pavlov's dictum.

    REFERENCES

    Campione,J

     and Brown, A.

     (1987).

     Linking dynamic assessment with

    school achievement. In Lidz, C.

     (ed.),

     D ynamic Assessment, Guil-

    ford  Press, New

     York,

     pp. 82-115.

    Carlson, J., and Das, J. P. (1997). A process approach to remediating word

    decoding deficiencies in Chapter 1 children.  Learning Disability

    Quarterly 20: 93-102.

    Cicerone,

     K. D., and

     Tupper,

     D. E, (1986).

     Cognitive assessment

      in the

    neuropsychological rehabilitation of head-injured adults. In Uzzell,

    B. P., and  Gross,  Y. (eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology  o f Interven-

    tion,

     Martinus

      N i j h o f f

    Boston.

    Clark,

     C. M  (1981).

     Statistical models

      and

     their application

      in

      clinical

    neuropsychological research and practice. In Filskov, S. B., and

    Boll,

     T, J.  (eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neuropsychology,

      Wiley,

    New York.

    Das, J. P.

     (1976).

     Psychological

      science:

     Some impressions of the

     scene

    in Moscow.

      Canadian Psychological Review

     17:151-157.

    Das,  J. P. (1988).  Simultaneous-Successive processing  and  planning.

    In

      Schmeck, R.

      (ed.), Learning Styles and Learning Strategies,

    Plenum,

     New

     York,

     pp.

     101-129.

    Das, J. P. (1992). Beyond a unidimensional scale of merit. Intelligence

    16:137-149.

    Das, J. P.

     (1994).

     Luria. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.),

     E ncyclopedia

      o f Human

    Intelligence,

     Macmillan, New York, pp.

     678-681.

    Das, J. P. (1995). Is there  life  afte r phonological coding?

      Issues in E du-

    cation: C ontributions from Educational Psychology  1, 87-90.

    Das, J. P.

     (1999).  P A S S  Reading Enhancement Program,

     Sarka Educa-

    tional Resources,

      Deal, NJ.

    Das, J. P., and Jarman, R. F.

     (1991).

     Cognitive integration: An alternative

    model for intelligence. In Rowe, H. (ed.), Intelligence: Reconcep-

    tualization

     and Measurement,

     Erlbaum,

     New

     York.

    Das,

      J. P.,

     Kar,

     B. C., and

     Parrila,

      R.

      (1996).

     Cognitive Planning: The

    Psychological  Basis  of  Intelligent Behavior,

      Sage,

      New  Delhi,

    India.

    Das, J. P., Kirby, J. R., and Jarman, R. F.

      (1975).

      Simultaneous and

    successive synthesis:

      An

     alternative model

      for

     cognitive

      abilities.

    Psychological

     Bulletin 82: 87-103.

    Das,

      J. P.,

     Kirby,

      J. R., and

      Jarman,

      R. F.

      (1979).

      Simultaneous and

    Successive C ognitive Processes,

     Academic

     Press,

     New York.

    Das, J. P., and Mishra, R. K. (1991).  Relation between memory span,

    naming time,

      speech rate,  and  reading

      competence.

      Journal of

    Experimental Education 59:129-139.

    Das, J. P., Mishra, R. K., and Kirby, J. R. (1994). Cognitive patterns of

    children with dyslexia: Comparison between groups with high

     and

    average nonverbal intelligence.

      Journal  o f Learning Disabilities

     4:

    235-242,253,

    Das,  J. P., Mishra,  R. K., and Pool,  J. E.

      (1995).

      An  experiment  on

    cognitive remediation

     of

     word-reading

     d i f fi cu l t y .  Journal o f Learn-

    in g

     Disabilities 28:66-79.

    Das, J. P., Naglieri, J. A., and Kirby, J. R.  (1994).  Assessment

      of

    Cognitive

     Processes,

     Allyn

     &

     Bacon,

     New

     York.

    Dean,

      R. S.  (1985).

      Foundation

      and

     rationale

      for

      neuropsychological

    bases  of

      individual differences.

      In

      Haulage

      L. C, and

      Telgrow,

    C. F.  (eds.),

      The Neuropsychology  of  Individual  D i f f e r e n c e s :   A

    Developmental

      Perspective, Plenum, New York.

    Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., and Lawrence, A.  (1984).  Speech

    rate

      and the

     development

      of

      short-term memory span.

      Journal of

    Experimental  Child Psychology

      38: 241-253.

    Kirby, J. R., and

     Das,

     J. P.

     (1990).

      A

     cognitive approach

     to intelligence:

    Attention, coding

      and

      planning.

      Canadian Psychology 31: 320-

    331.

    Kuhn,  D.

     (1995).

      Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us?

    Psychological

     Science

     6:113-139.

    Luria,  A. R.

      (1963).  Restoration  of  Functions

      A f t e r

      Brain

      I n j u r y

    Macmillan,

      New

     York.

    Luria, A. R.

     (1966a). Higher Cortical Functions in M an,

      Basic Books,

    New York.

    Luria, A. R.

      (1966b).

      Human Brain and Psychological Processes,

    Harper

      &

     Row,

     New

     York.

    Luria, A. R.

     (1973). T he

      W o r k i n g

      Brain: An Introduction to Neuropsy-

    chology,

     Basic Books, New York.

    Luria,

      A. R., and

      Tsvetkova,

      L. S.  (1990). The Neuropsychological

    Analysis   o f Problem Solving,

     Paul M. Deutsch Press, Orlando, FL.

    McArdle,

     J. J.  (1996).

     Current directions

      in

      structural factor analysis.

    Current

     Directions 5: 11-18.

    Naglieri,

     J. A. (1989). A

     cognitive processing theory

     for the

     measurement

    of  intelligence.

     Educational Psychologist 24:185-206.

    Naglieri, J. A., and Das, J. P. (1997). D as-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment

    System,

     Riverside Publishers, Itasca.

    Naglieri, J. A., Prewett, P., and Bardos, A. N.  (1989).  An explora-

    tory  study for planning, attention, simultaneous and successive

    cognitive

      processes.  Journal of School Psychology  27:  347-

    364.

    Naglieri, J. A., and Reardon, S. M. (1993). Traditional IQ is irrelevant

    to

      learning disabilities—Intelligence  is not

      Journal

      of

     Learning

    Disabilities 26: 127-133.

    Pavlov,

      I. P. (1928).

      Lectures

     on

      Conditioned  R e f l e x e s

      (W. H. Grant,

    trans.), International Publishers, New York.

    Posner, M. I., and Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention.

    Psychological Review 78: 391-408.

    Salomon,

      G., and

      Perkins,

      D.  (1989).

      Rocky roads

      to

      transfer:

      Re-

    thinking mechanisms

      of a

      neglected phenomenon.

      Educational

    Psychologist 24:113-142.

    Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning.  Philosophical

    Transactions

      o f

      the Royal Society o f London 298:199-209.

  • 8/15/2019 Aproximación neo-Luriana Diagnostico Rehabilitación

    10/10

    116 Das

    Share,

      D., and

      Stanovich,

     K. E.

      1995).

      Cognitive processes

      in

      early

    reading development: Accommodating individual differences into

    a model of acquisition.

      Issues

      in

     E ducation: C ontributions from

    Educational

     Psychology

      1:1-57.

    Siegler, R. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cogni-

    tive development. C urrent D irections in Psychological Science

     3:

    1-4.

    Spencer,

     F.,

     Snart,

     F., and

     Das,

     I. P. (1989). A

     process-based approach

    to

     the

     remediation

     of

     spelling

      in

     students with reading disabilities.

    T h e  Alberta Journal

     o f E ducational

     R esearch

     35:269-282.

    Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic

    and

     the

     garden-variety poor reader:

     The

     phonological-core variable

    difference

      model. Journal

     of

     Learning Disabilities

     21: 590-604,

    612.

    Stroop,

      J. R.

     (1935).  Studies

     of

      interference

      in

     serial verbal reactions.

    Journal  o f  E xperimental Psychology  18:

     643-661.

    Torgesen, J. K., Kistner ,J

    .,

     and Morgan, S. (1987). Component processes

    in working memory. In Borkowski, J., and Day, J. (eds.),

      Memory

    and

      Cognition

     in  Special

      C hildren: Perspectives

     on

     Retardation,

    Learning

     D isabilities

     and G iftedness, Ablex Publishing, Norwood,

    NJ, pp. 49-86.

    Vygotsky,  L. S.  (1962).

      Thought

      and

      Language,

      M.I.T.

      Press,

    Cambridge, MA.

    Walsh, K. W. (1985).

      Understanding  Brain D amage,

      Churchill

    Livingstone, London.

    Walsh, K. W. (1987). Neuropsychology:

      A

     C linical Approach, Churchill

    Livingstone, London.

    Woodcock, R. W, (1987).

      W o o d c o c k

      Reading

     M astery Test— Revised,

    American

     Guidance Service, Circle Pines,

      MN.

    Zeigarnik, B. V., Luria, A. R., and Polyakov, Y. F. (1977). On the use of

    psychological tests in clinical practice in the USSR.

     I ntelligence

    1:82-93.